Chronobiol Med Search

CLOSE


Chronobiol Med > Volume 7(2); 2025 > Article
Oshita, Ishihara, Seike, Myotsuzono, and Imai: Associations Between Weekday/Weekend Sleep Duration Differences, Chronotype, and Physical Activity in Female University Students

Abstract

Objective

The abundance of digital devices has raised concerns about the impact of nighttime use of these devices on daily rhythms, especially for evening-type individuals. Therefore, a simple assessment method for chronotypes is required. This study investigated the associations between weekday-to-weekend sleep duration (WWD), chronotype, and physical activity level (PAL) in female university students.

Methods

Participants were 201 female university students. PAL and sleep duration on weekdays and weekends were assessed from daily activity diary records. Chronotype was assessed using the Morningness–Eveningness Questionnaire (MEQ).

Results

WWD exhibited weak negative and strong positive correlations with sleep duration on weekdays and weekends, respectively. Furthermore, the WWD had a moderate relationship with the MEQ score. These results indicate that individuals with the evening chronotype may compensate for the sleep deficit on weekdays by sleeping longer on weekends, which was reflected in the WWD. Further, although the WWD was not correlated with the PAL on weekdays, it was negatively correlated with the PAL on weekends. Therefore, sleeping for longer on the weekend indicates a shorter duration of other daytime activities, such as physical activity.

Conclusion

The WWD can be assessed with a simple question and may be useful as a means of assessing approximate chronotypes without any notable burden on participants.

INTRODUCTION

Mobile phones have become an integral part of our lives and have notably impacted society; mobile phone penetration rates exceed 100% in most developed countries [1]. One of the problems associated with mobile phones is their impact on sleep and daily rhythms. For example, the National Health and Nutrition Survey in Japan reported that approximately half of respondents in their 20s answered that mobile phone use, texting, and games disturbed their sleep [2]. Furthermore, a recent systematic review and meta-analysis suggested that excessive smartphone use was closely associated with poor self-reported sleep quality, sleep deprivation, and sleep latency prolongation [3]. Another study found that smartphone screen time, mainly mediated by bedtime use, delayed circadian rhythms and reduced total sleep time [4]. In particular, the chronotype, that is, the time orientation of an individual, is known to be the latest in one’s life (becoming the eveningness type [ET]) among young adults around the age of 20 years [5]. As digital devices (e.g., computer screens, mobile devices, and smartphones) are often reported to be related to chronotype (i.e., smartphone use is related to the lateness of the chronotype) in young adults, including university students [6-10], young adults whose chronotype becomes more ET will be more susceptible to a society with an abundance of digital devices.
Most people spend their lives in a morning-oriented society, which forces ET individuals to adopt a morning lifestyle contrary to their time orientation. Therefore, ET individuals experience sleep deficits on weekdays [11] because they are forced to wake up earlier to adapt to a morning-oriented society, even if their bedtime is later. In particular, young adults oversleep on free days (i.e., weekends) to compensate for the sleep deficit accumulated over weekdays because of their late chronotype [12]. Oversleeping on weekends is referred to as weekend catch-up sleep (WeCUS). Furthermore, such differences in sleep between work/school days and free days are assessed by the difference in the midpoint of sleep (MS) (described later) between work/school days (MSW) and free days (MSF) and are called social jet-lag (SJL) [13]. In a society with an abundance of digital devices, a previous study found that having electronic devices in the bedroom and using the internet before going to bed are associated with SJL [14]. Increasing evidence suggests that SJL is a health risk factor (particularly affecting metabolic and circulatory functions). It predisposes patients to diseases later in life [15]. In particular, females may be at a higher risk of developing adverse health symptoms because they have a higher SJL than males [12]. Therefore, issues related to chronotypes and SJL are important in young females.
Additionally, a recent report showed that an increase in SJL was linked to less physical activity (PA) in female students [16]. Lower physical activity levels (PALs) are believed to lead to an imbalance in body composition (lower muscle mass and higher body fat). A more recent study reported that higher body fat and lower muscle mass were significantly associated with chronotype lateness and lower PAL in female university students [17]. A previous study on mobile use among university students reported that smartphone use or dependence may be related to chronotypes [9]. Thus, current digital society requires a simplified assessment of chronotypes in young females, and measures should be taken, especially in the case of ET individuals.
Chronotypes are typically assessed using self-reported questionnaires. For example, the Morningness–Eveningness Questionnaire (MEQ) [18] evaluates chronotypes by calculating a score from answers to 19 questions. Others have used MS to represent the clock time midway between sleep onset (falling asleep) and offset (waking up). The SJL evaluates the difference between the MSW and MSF [13]. Furthermore, the Munich Chronotype Questionnaire (MCTQ) [19] was used to assess 17 simple questions based on the MSF and sleep duration on weekdays and weekends. However, as MSF is assessed using spontaneous sleep, it cannot be evaluated when sleep is affected by external constraints, such as an alarm clock. It is also impossible to evaluate when there is no regular work or school or when there are no regular free days. Therefore, other studies have evaluated the variation in sleep duration over a week and the time at sleep onset and offset using the standard deviation (SD) [20] or the coefficient of variation (CV) [21,22]. In particular, as the SJL or SD of sleep duration cannot evaluate sleep duration itself, the CV of sleep duration is also considered to be able to assess sleep duration as well [21,22]. However, the SD was calculated as a positive (absolute) value. This means that sleeping longer on weekends than on weekdays and sleeping longer on weekdays than on weekends were processed equally. WeCUS is an attempt to catch up on weekday sleep debt on weekends (mostly ET individuals). Conversely, if individuals get enough sleep on weekdays (mainly individuals of the morning type [MT]), their sleep duration may not be prolonged or may be shortened on weekends. If differences in sleep duration were evaluated using SD, they were evaluated equally. A study on college students reported that a few (6%) of them slept less on weekends than on weekdays [23]. Therefore, although one approach is to correct for MSF using average sleep duration across the entire week and oversleeping on weekends, this only applies to individuals who sleep longer on free days than on workdays [24].
Considering these problems, a simple assessment of the differences in sleep duration between weekdays and weekends may be sufficient to evaluate the approximate chronotype. In this case, instead of using several questions or examining the everyday sleep duration, simple questions could be asked, “How long do you sleep on weekdays (school/work days)?” and “How long do you sleep on your weekends (days when you do not have plans)?” are all that is needed. Furthermore, if sleep duration was considered, the ratio of the mean sleep duration on weekdays and weekends (minutes) to the difference in sleep duration on weekdays and weekends (minutes) could be calculated using these two questions. Therefore, this study investigated the association between differences in sleep duration on weekdays and weekends (weekday-to-weekend sleep duration [WWD]) and chronotype, as measured by the MEQ score, as well as the association with PAL in female university students.

METHODS

Participants

Of the 213 total female university students, 201 participants were included in the analysis, excluding 2 with a very high PAL (2.4 or higher, PAL estimation described below) on weekday or weekend and 10 with incomplete survey responses. Participants were recruited from several universities in different prefectures in Japan, and their academic years were randomly chosen (aged 18–22 years). For ethical considerations, participants were informed orally and in writing in advance that the survey would be anonymous, that it would be used for the purpose of this study and not for any other purpose, and that any data not used for the study would be discarded. In the event that the results of the survey were published, participants were told that the collected data would be statistically processed and then published in such a way that individuals could not be identified, and that the survey would only be carried out if participants consented. The Research Ethics Committee of Kyushu Kyoritsu University approved this study (approval number: 2022-08).

Survey of daily life activities

The average daily activity and exercise times on weekdays and weekends in the previous month were evaluated using a daily activity diary. The participants were instructed to report the average on weekdays when they had university (i.e., average on school days), and on weekends, when they had no plans (free days). The PAL of each activity was evaluated by calculating the daily average of each classified activity using the product of the energy expenditure index, expressed as a multiple of the basal metabolic rate (BMR) and activity time. This method was based on a study that estimated PAL according to the lifestyle of university students, which was based on the “Dietary Reference Intakes for Japanese People” [25]. Furthermore, the PALs on weekdays and holidays were assessed. Sleep duration was obtained from the responses, and the difference in sleep duration between weekdays and weekends was evaluated as the WWD. The %WWD was also calculated as WWD divided by the average sleep duration on weekdays and weekends, as shown in the following equation:
%WWD=WWD(min)Mean sleep duration on weekday and weekend(min)×100
In addition, the SD of sleep duration on weekdays and weekends (sleep-SD) was calculated.

Chronotype

The Japanese version [26] of Horne and Ostberg’s MEQ [18] was used to assess chronotypes. Individuals exhibiting a chronotype were classified as definitely MT (score: 70–86), moderately MT (score: 59–69), neither type (score: 42–58), moderately ET (score: 31–41), or definitely ET (score: 16–30) [18].

Statistical analysis

The mean and SD of each variable were calculated for all participants. The relationship between WWD or %WWD and MEQ scores was examined using regression analysis to determine the regression equations and coefficients of determination. Pearson’s correlation analysis was used to examine the correlation coefficients between WWD and other variables (weekday and weekend sleep duration and PAL). The StatFlex statistical software (ver. 7.0.10; Artec) was used for these statistical analyses, with a statistical significance level of p<0.05. Regarding evaluation of the strength of the association, for absolute values of r, 0.00–0.19 is regarded as very weak, 0.20–0.39 as weak, 0.40–0.59 as moderate, 0.60–0.79 as strong, and 0.80–1.00 as very strong correlation [27].

RESULTS

Table 1 shows the means and SDs of the measured variables for all participants. Figure 1 shows the frequency distribution of the MEQ scores. The mean was 46, whereas the median was 45, indicating a slightly biased distribution toward lower values.
Figure 2 shows the relationship between Sleep-SD and WWD. A V-shaped linear relationship between Sleep-SD and WWD is shown, with the WWD bordering zero.
Figure 3 shows the relationship between the WWD and sleep duration on weekdays (left) and weekends (right). The WWD was significantly correlated with both sleep durations, with a weak negative relationship with weekday sleep duration and a strong positive relationship with weekend sleep duration. This indicated that individuals with longer sleep durations on weekends compared to weekdays had shorter sleep durations on weekdays and longer sleep durations on weekends. WWD showed no significant correlation with weekday PAL (r=0.12, p=0.08, 95% confidence interval [CI]=-0.02 to 0.26), whereas a significant weak negative correlation with PAL on weekends (r=-0.23, p<0.01, 95% CI=-0.36 to -0.10). This indicates that individuals with longer sleep duration on weekdays compared to weekdays had less PA on weekends.
Figure 4 shows the relationship between MEQ score, WWD, and %WWD. The MEQ score showed a significant moderate relationship with WWD and %WWD (R=-0.40 and -0.41, respectively). Although the regression equations were MEQ score=-0.0345×WWD+51.434 for WWD and MEQ score=-0.1662×%WWD+51.711 for %WWD, the coefficients of determination were only 0.16 and 0.17. This indicates that the MEQ score can be explained by WWD and %WWD.

DISCUSSION

The average sleep duration of the study participants was 376.2 minutes on weekdays. This was almost the same as the mean value for Japanese females in an international study that investigated the mean sleep duration of university students (6.09 h) [28]. As shown in Figure 2, both SD-Sleep and WWD were calculated using two variables (sleep duration on weekdays and weekends), which resulted in a nearly linear relationship. However, as SD sleep duration had only positive (absolute) values, individuals who slept longer on weekdays than on weekends and vice versa were same SD-Sleep. This is a completely natural result, indicating that when the variation in sleep duration is evaluated by SD, the values of those who sleep longer on weekdays than on weekends are not reflected in the results.
The WWD exhibited a weak negative correlation with weekday sleep duration and a strong positive correlation with weekend sleep duration (Figure 3). This result indicates that WWD is longer for those who sleep shorter on weekdays and longer on weekends. While ET individuals go to bed later, they must adapt their weekday waking time to that of the morning society because of social restrictions. Consequently, they experience sleep deficits on weekdays. Furthermore, previous studies reported that individuals sleep longer on weekends to compensate for sleep deficits [12]. A study of male Japanese students found that the difference in sleep duration between weekdays and weekends was significantly related to MSF [22]. The present study also found that both WWD and %WWD had a significant linear relationship with the MEQ score, and the regression coefficients (R) were almost equal (-0.40 and -0.41), indicating a moderate relationship. These results indicate that individuals with ET sleep for more hours on weekends than on weekdays. Particularly, WWD in this study was weakly related to weekday sleep duration; however, strongly related to weekend sleep duration, suggesting that WWD reflects weekend sleep duration more than weekday sleep duration.
Longer sleep duration on weekends indicates shorter duration of other activities on weekends. This study showed that, although WWD was not significantly related to PAL on weekdays, it was significantly negatively related to PAL on weekends. A previous review indicated that the association between PA and chronotypes among university students was inconsistent [29]. One reason for this inconsistency may be the difference in PA between weekdays and weekends. On weekdays, students go to campus, move from one classroom to another, and engage in after-school activities. This results in a certain amount of PAL regardless of the chronotype or sleep duration. However, on weekends, some students may be active in the morning, whereas others may be inactive. Therefore, the WWD in this study may have been significantly related only to the PAL on weekends. Some reports on female university students have found that PAL is related to body composition, including muscle mass [25], whereas others have found no such relationship [30]. These results suggest that surveys of PALs, especially for university students, should consider the differences between weekdays and weekends.
This study examined whether WWD or %WWD could represent the chronotype, as assessed using the MEQ. A moderately significant linear relationship was found between WWD or %WWD and the MEQ score; however, the coefficient of determination (R2) was approximately 0.16 to 0.17. The WWD in this study was assessed using the self-reported sleep durations of the participants, that is, their approximate sleeping duration on weekdays and weekends. Therefore, several sleep durations in this study were considered different from the actual sleep duration. Therefore, it may not be surprising that the WWD assessed in these studies can only explain the chronotype. However, the MEQ questionnaire survey requires 19 questions to be answered and the total value to be calculated. The SD and CV of sleep duration also require recording in a sleep diary for several consecutive days. Conversely, WWD in this study could be evaluated using only two simple questions: “How long do you sleep on weekdays?” and “How long do you sleep on weekends (free days)?” Surveys on sleep duration are often included in lifestyle questionnaires. In addition, mobile phones are highly prevalent, as indicated in the introduction, and some applications exist for measuring sleep duration on smartphones. This can be used to evaluate WWD automatically. Therefore, the WWD may be useful for assessing approximate chronotypes without any notable burden on participants.
This study had several limitations. First, because the survey was based on a questionnaire, the actual sleep duration needs to be investigated. In addition, activity time should also be assessed objectively using an activity meter. Second, as SJL is a problem in individuals with ET, future studies should include SJL. Furthermore, some individuals take naps to compensate for their sleep debt [31]. Indeed, previous research suggests that napping may be the most adaptive and efficient way to deal with sleep debt [32]. However, this study only assessed sleep duration, and the state of napping is unknown. Therefore, future studies should include the details of napping. Finally, although this study was conducted on female university students as indicated in the Introduction, male students and other generations also need to be examined.
This study investigated the association between differences in sleep duration on weekdays and weekends and chronotype, as measured by the MEQ score, as well as the association with PAL in female university students. The results indicated that difference in sleep duration had a moderate negative relationship with the MEQ score. Moreover, difference in sleep duration exhibited a weak negative correlation with sleep duration on weekdays and a strong positive correlation with sleep duration on weekends. Therefore, ET individuals may compensate for the sleep deficit on weekdays by sleeping longer on weekends, and this was reflected in the difference in sleep duration. Further, since longer sleep on weekends shortens the duration of other activities, a significant negative relationship between difference in sleep duration and PAL on weekends was also observed. These results suggest that WWD can be evaluated with simple questions; “How long do you sleep on weekdays (school days)?” and “How long do you sleep on weekends (have no plans)?” and it may be useful as a means of assessing approximate chronotypes without a significant burden on the participants.

NOTES

Conflicts of Interest

The authors have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

Availability of Data and Material

The datasets generated and analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: Kazushige Oshita. Data curation: all authors. Formal analysis: all authors. Funding acquisition: Kazushige Oshita, Yujiro Ishihara, Kohei Seike, Ryota Myotsuzono. Investigation: Kazushige Oshita, Yujiro Ishihara, Kohei Seike, Ryota Myotsuzono. Methodology: all authors. Project administration: Kazushige Oshita. Supervision: Yujiro Ishihara, Kohei Seike, Ryota Myotsuzono. Validation: all authors. Visualization: Kazushige Oshita. Writing—original draft: Kazushige Oshita. Writing—review & editing: all authors.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by the Wesco Science Promotion Foundation.

Acknowledgments

None

Figure 1.
Frequency distribution of MEQ scores. MEQ, Morningness–Eveningness Questionnaire.
cim-2025-0013f1.jpg
Figure 2.
Relationship between Sleep-SD and the WWD. Sleep-SD, standard deviation of sleep duration on weekdays and weekends; WWD, weekday-to-weekend sleep duration.
cim-2025-0013f2.jpg
Figure 3.
Relationship between WWD and weekday and weekend sleep duration. WWD, weekday-to-weekend sleep duration; CI, confidence interval.
cim-2025-0013f3.jpg
Figure 4.
Relationship between MEQ score and WWD and %WWD. MEQ, Morningness–Eveningness Questionnaire; WWD, weekday-to-weekend sleep duration; %WWD, WWD divided by the average sleep duration on weekdays and weekends.
cim-2025-0013f4.jpg
Table 1.
Key measured parameters of the participants (n=201)
Parameter Value (mean±SD)
Age (yr) 19±1
Height (cm) 157.8±12.4
Weight (kg) 53.9±9.4
BMI (kg/m2) 21.6±3.2
MEQ score 46±9
Sleep duration weekday (min) 376.2±81.3
Sleep duration weekend (min) 529.7±111.3
WWD (min) 153.5±104.5
PAL weekday (min) 1.77±0.35
PAL weekend (min) 1.65±0.35

BMI, body mass index; MEQ, Morningness–Eveningness Questionnaire; WWD, weekday-to-weekend sleep duration; PAL, physical activity level; SD, standard deviation

REFERENCES

1. Birenboim A, Shoval N. Mobility research in the age of the smartphone. Ann Am Assoc Geogr 2016;106:283–291.
crossref
2. Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare. The national health and nutrition survey in Japan [Internet]. Tokyo: Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare; 2020. Available at: https://www.mhlw.go.jp/content/001066903.pdf. Accessed November 1, 2024.

3. Chu Y, Oh Y, Gwon M, Hwang S, Jeong H, Kim HW, et al. Dose-response analysis of smartphone usage and self-reported sleep quality: a systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. J Clin Sleep Med 2023;19:621–630.
crossref pmid pmc
4. Lin YH, Wong BY, Lin SH, Chiu YC, Pan YC, Lee YH. Development of a mobile application (app) to delineate “digital chronotype” and the effects of delayed chronotype by bedtime smartphone use. J Psychiatr Res 2019;110:9–15.
crossref pmid
5. Roenneberg T, Kuehnle T, Pramstaller PP, Ricken J, Havel M, Guth A, et al. A marker for the end of adolescence. Curr Biol 2004;14:R1038–R1039.
crossref pmid
6. Demirhan E, Randler C, Horzum MB. Is problematic mobile phone use explained by chronotype and personality? Chronobiol Int 2016;33:821–831.
crossref pmid
7. Eugenia T, Paulina S, Justyna M. Disruption of circadian rhythmicity–an exploratory study in Poland. Pers Individ Differ 2014;66:98–101.
crossref
8. Randjelovic P, Stojiljkovic N, Radulovic N, Stojanovic N, Ilic I. Problematic smartphone use, screen time and chronotype correlations in university students. Eur Addict Res 2021;27:67–74.
crossref pmid pdf
9. Toda M, Nishio N, Takeshita T. Predictive factors for smartphone dependence: relationship to demographic characteristics, chronotype, and depressive state of university students. Open J Prev Med 2015;5:456–462.
crossref pdf
10. Toda M, Nishio N, Ezoe S, Takeshita T. Chronotype and smartphone use among Japanese medical students. Int J Cyber Behav Psychol Learn 2015;5:75–80.
crossref
11. Roenneberg T, Kuehnle T, Juda M, Kantermann T, Allebrandt K, Gordijn M, et al. Epidemiology of the human circadian clock. Sleep Med Rev 2007;11:429–438.
crossref pmid
12. Komada Y, Okajima I, Kitamura S, Inoue Y. A survey on social jetlag in Japan: a nationwide, cross-sectional internet survey. Sleep Biol Rhythms 2019;17:417–422.
crossref pdf
13. Wittmann M, Dinich J, Merrow M, Roenneberg T. Social jetlag: misalignment of biological and social time. Chronobiol Int 2006;23:497–509.
crossref pmid
14. Lang CJ, Reynolds AC, Appleton SL, Taylor AW, Gill TK, McEvoy RD, et al. Sociodemographic and behavioural correlates of social jetlag in Australian adults: results from the 2016 National Sleep Health Foundation Study. Sleep Med 2018;51:133–139.
crossref pmid
15. Caliandro R, Streng AA, van Kerkhof LWM, van der Horst GTJ, Chaves I. Social jetlag and related risks for human health: a timely review. Nutrients 2021;13:4543.
crossref pmid pmc
16. Shibata M, Fujibayashi M, Shibata S, Kuzuhara K, Tanida K. Association between social jetlag and objective physical activity among female university students of Japan: a cross-sectional study. Sleep Sci 2024;17:e151–e156.
crossref pmid pmc
17. Oshita K, Ishihara Y, Seike K, Myotsuzono R. Associations of body composition with physical activity, nutritional intake status, and chronotype among female university students in Japan. J Physiol Anthropol 2024;43:13.
crossref pmid pmc pdf
18. Horne JA, Ostberg O. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol 1976;4:97–110.
pmid
19. Roenneberg T, Wirz-Justice A, Merrow M. Life between clocks: daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes. J Biol Rhythms 2003;18:80–90.
crossref pmid pdf
20. Fischer D, Klerman EB, Phillips AJK. Measuring sleep regularity: theoretical properties and practical usage of existing metrics. Sleep 2021;44:zsab103.
crossref pmid pmc pdf
21. Nishimura K, Tamari Y, Yamaguchi H, Onodera S, Nagasaki K. [The relationship between variation in time of waking up and going to bed for a week and social jetlag in young men]. Jpn J Biometeorol 2022;59:15–24.Japanese.

22. Nishimura K, Tamari Y, Yamaguchi H, Onodera S, Nagasaki K. Examination of sleep factors affecting social jetlag in Japanese male college students. Chronobiol Int 2023;40:192–198.
crossref pmid
23. Vestergaard CL, Simpson MR, Sivertsen B, Kallestad H, Langsrud K, Scott J, et al. Weekday-to-weekend sleep duration patterns among young adults and outcomes related to health and academic performance. Sleep Sci Pract 2024;8:15.
crossref pdf
24. Roenneberg T, Pilz LK, Zerbini G, Winnebeck EC. Chronotype and social jetlag: a (self-) critical review. Biology (Basel) 2019;8:54.
crossref pmid pmc
25. Oshita K, Myotsuzono R. An association between the physical activity level and skeletal muscle mass index in female university students with a past exercise habituation. Osteoporos Sarcopenia 2021;7:146–152.
crossref pmid pmc
26. Ishihara K, Saitoh T, Inoue Y, Miyata Y. Validity of the Japanese version of the morningness-eveningness questionnaire. Percept Mot Skills 1984;59:863–866.
crossref pdf
27. Swinscow TDV, Campbell MJ. Statistics at square one, 9th ed. London, BMJ Publishing. 1996.

28. Steptoe A, Peacey V, Wardle J. Sleep duration and health in young adults. Arch Intern Med 2006;166:1689–1692.
crossref pmid
29. Sempere-Rubio N, Aguas M, Faubel R. Association between chronotype, physical activity and sedentary behaviour: a systematic review. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2022;19:9646.
crossref pmid pmc
30. Kusakabe T, Arai H, Yamamoto Y, Nakao K, Akamatsu Y, Ishihara Y, et al. Cross-sectional association of skeletal muscle mass and strength with dietary habits and physical activity among first-year university students in Japan: results from the KEIJI-U study. Nutrition 2024;118:112265.
crossref pmid
31. Leger D, Richard JB, Collin O, Sauvet F, Faraut B. Napping and weekend catchup sleep do not fully compensate for high rates of sleep debt and short sleep at a population level (in a representative nationwide sample of 12,637 adults). Sleep Med 2020;74:278–288.
crossref pmid
32. Faraut B, Andrillon T, Vecchierini MF, Leger D. Napping: a public health issue. From epidemiological to laboratory studies. Sleep Med Rev 2017;35:85–100.
crossref pmid
TOOLS
Share :
Facebook Twitter Linked In Google+ Line it
METRICS Graph View
  • 0 Crossref
  •   Scopus 
  • 131 View
  • 4 Download
Related articles in Chronobiol Med


ABOUT
ARTICLE CATEGORY

Browse all articles >

BROWSE ARTICLES
EDITORIAL POLICIES
FOR CONTRIBUTORS
Editorial Office
RN1611, 725, Suseo-Dong, Gangnam-Gu, Seoul 06367, Republic of Korea
Tel: +82-2-445-1611    Fax: +82-2-445-1633    E-mail: editor@chronobiologyinmedicine.org                

Copyright © 2025 by Korean Academy of Sleep Medicine. All rights reserved.

Developed in M2PI

Close layer
prev next